The Weak Suffer What They Must — A Natural Experiment in Thought and Structure

The contrast between Schwab in 1996 (Figure 2) and 1998 (Figure 4) shows the structural change that Suzan’s story illustrates. In the corresponding 1996 and 1998 proximity matrices illustrated in tables 6 and 7 show the contrast as members see stewards and reps as closer to managers and supervisors in 1998 than in 1996, see themselves as closer to managers but a somewhat less close to supervisors, and see themselves as less similar to reps and stewards. In these respects, the 1998 proximity matrix for Schwab resembles the industrial one.

To what extent do these figures and tables represent shared patterns of thought? This has been an issue since Wallace raised it (1961). Romney, Weller, and Batchelder (1988) outline the theory and mathematics for computing respondents’ consensus on cultural domains, and Borgatti (1996a,b) includes it in Anthropac. Here, the purpose of using consensus analysis is only to assess the agreement among respondents. The procedure computes a factor analysis of the chance-adjusted measures of agreement among respondents. If the first factor is less than three times as great as the second, there is no consensus. Table 8 shows the ratio of the first to the second factor for each group and the number of respondents. It is clear that there is sufficient consensus at each site to consider the proximity matrices cultural constructs.

Given that there is consensus among respondents, to what extent are the samples of respondents representative of the whole group? My sampling techniques were neither very consistent nor very pure. The exigencies of shifts, transportation across a wide area, dubious quality of membership lists-the only available sampling frame–and other factors made the random sampling that I desired and attempted hopelessly quixotic. To do the work, I had to rely on other techniques, chiefly opportunistic sampling. Some of the samples represent almost the entire population, and for those sampling is not problematic. Virtually the entire staff of the local responded, so that is not an issue. Almost all of the non-support staff of the second local responded, and that included virtually all of the reps. Schwab has about 60 members (64 in 1996, but the number varies as Suzan’s narrative in this paper indicates); the first sample of 6 is thus about 10%; the second is nearly half, 42% and represents all departments as does the first sample. While these are neither random nor stratified samples, they are fairly representative of the membership. The industrial sample is from a random sample list of four industrial sites I drew from the membership list. This represents about 1% of the total industrial membership of the local. The public hospital sample partially from a random sample list I drew from the membership list and partially an opportunistic sample of its 674 service employees and 79 technicians (753 total), about a 1% sample. I think the samples of the staffs and Schwab members are representative; there may be some doubt about the industrial and public hospital samples, but here the sampling was more purely random.

Structure, agency, and class
Alford (1998) points out the problems of the multivariate approach in understanding historical phenomena–dependent variables may become independent variables as systems of relationships change over time. In highly stratified social orders defined by differential access to productive resources (Fried 1967), agency and structure may have different positions in different classes because access to resources, hence class position, rests on differential power, a structural dimension given by position in the social order. Among the working class in the United States there seems to be dual awareness of both structure and agency whereas middle class persons think more in terms of agency than structure. This suggests that the concern anthropologists have with the topic of agency may be a projection of our own class-based folk models onto the rest of the world (Durrenberger and Erem 1997b). Working class awareness of structures is less a cultural convention than a recognition of the reality of powerlessness (Newman 1988, 1993; Hunt 1996; Hackenberg 1995; Griffith 1995; Rubin 1992).

The relationship has never been stated more clearly than in the words Thucydides put into the mouths of the Athenian delegation to Melos in the sixteenth year of the Peloponnesian War: “. . . you know as well as we do that right, as the world goes, is only in question between equals in power, while the strong do what they can and the weak suffer what they must” (Crawley nd: 359). They went on to make the nomothetic observation, “Of the gods we believe, and of men we know, that by a necessary law of their nature they rule wherever they can,” elaborating that the did not make this law, but found it “and shall leave it to exist for ever after us. . . .” (Crawley nd: 361-362). The democratic Anthenians then besieged the place and finally, after the Melians surrendered, killed all of the men and sold the women and children into slavery (Crawley nd:365). The Athenians’ observations are as valid for modern social orders as for ancient ones.

The chief goal of the union movement in industrial social orders is to redress the structural imbalance and give some sense of agency to those who provide labor but do not necessarily control the conditions for its use. It does this by attempting to develop collective power based on other structural principals than wealth. The intentions of the powerful are more consequential than those of the un-powerful. Thus by the actions of the powerful do their intentions become structures that determine the cognition of the powerless. Cognition and agency may determine structure for some classes while for others structure may determine cognition; the causal arrows may be functions of class position or power rather than being constant across the whole social order. Thus even the question of causal relations between structure and cognition is not a constant but varys by class. This is one theoretically iportant reason that anthropologists should not perpetuate the American folk model of the classless society but explore the structural, cultural, and practical dimensions and cosnequences of stratification in our own (see the works in Forman 1995) and other social orders ancient and modern.

Epilogue
By the middle of the summer of 1998 we had everyone back up to 8 hours at Schwab and I had held meetings to train managers and supervisors to work with the stewards as they are required to by law. I’d trained stewards on how to be treated as equals by management) when they were used to being treated as subordinates–mutually agreed upon meetings (rather than just meeting when management demanded a meeting at a time and place management dictated), how to command meetings (rather than be a passive audience), how to show solidarity when they needed to (rather than each person only looking out for him or herself). With high turnover among management, new stewards and the law behind me, I have some power to create new structures. If management elects not to participate, they know I can file charges against them with the Labor Board and cause them more trouble than they want. More importantly, the members are regaining a sense of ownership in the union and of their own ability to affect what happens at their job as they become more organized and see the consequences of their concerted activity.

———

References Cited

Alford, Robert R.
The Craft of Inquiry: Theories, Methods, Evidence. New York: Oxford University Press.

Bernard, H. Russell
1988 Research Methods in Cultural Anthropology. Newbury Park: Sage.

Black, Mary B.
Eliciting Folk Taxonomy in Ojibwa. IN Cognitive Anthropology, Stephen Tylor, editor. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Pages 165-189.

Borgatti, Stephen P.
1996a Anthropac 4.0. Natick, MA: Analytic Technologies.
1996b Anthropac 4.0 Methods Guide. Natick, MA: Analytic Technologies.

Brown, Cecil H., John Kolar, Barbara J. Torrey, Tipawan Trúóng-Quang, and Phillip Volkman.
Some General Principles of Biological and Non-Biological Folk Classification. American Ethnologist 3(1):73-86.

Crawley, Richard, trans.
ND Thucydides: The Peloponnesian War. New York: Dolphin Books.

Durrenberger, E. Paul
1982 An analysis of Lisu symbolism, economics, and cognition. Pacific Viewpoint 23:127-145.
Gulf Coast Soundings: People and Policy in the Mississippi Shrimp Industry.
Lawrence: University Press of Kansas.
That’ll Teach You: Cognition and Practice in a Chicago Union Local. Human
Organization 56 (4)388-392.

Durrenberger, E. Paul and John Morrison
1979 Comments on Brown’s Ethnoscience. American Ethnologist 6(2):408-409.
Durrenberger, E. Paul and Suzan Erem
1997a Getting a Raise: Organizing Workers in an Industrializing Hospital. Journal of
Anthropological Research 53(1):31-46.
1997b The Dance of Power: Ritual and Agency among Unionized American
Health Care Workers. American Anthropologist 99 (3):489-495.

Erem, Suzan and E. Paul Durrenberger
1997 The Way I See It: Perspectives on the Labor Movement from the People in it.
Anthropology and Humanism. 22(2):159-169.

Forman, Shepard, editor
Diagnosing America: Anthropologly and Public Engagement. Ann Arbor:
University of Michigan Press.

Fried, Morton H.
1967 The Evolution of Political Society. New York: Random House.

Gatewood, John B.
Actions Speak Louder than Words. IN Directions in Cognitive Anthropology. Janet W.D. Daugherty, editor. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Pages 199-219.

Griffith, David
1995 Hay Trabajo: Poultry Processing, Rural Industrialization, and the Latinization of Low-Wage Labor. IN Any Way you Cut it: Meat Processing and Small-Town America. Edited by Donald D. Stull, Michael J. Broadway, and David Griffith. Lawrence: University Press of Kansas. Pages 129-151.

Hackenberg, Robert A.
Conclusion: Joe Hill Died for Your Sins: Empowering Minority Workers in the
New Industrial Labor Force. IN Any Way you Cut it: Meat Processing and Small-Town America. Edited by Donald D. Stull, Michael J. Broadway, and David Griffith. Lawrence: University Press of Kansas. Pages 231-264.

Hunt, Matthew O.
The Individual, Society, or Both: A Comparison of Black, Latino, and White
Beliefs about the Causes of Poverty. Social Forces 75 (1):293-322.

Kay, Paul
Comments on Colby. Comment on “Ethnographic Semantics: A Preliminary Survey by B.N. Colby. Current Anthropology 7 (1):20-23. Reprinted in Cognitive Anthropology, Stephen Tylor, editor. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Pages 78-90.

Keesing, R.
Paradigms Lost: The New Ethnography and the new Linguistics. Southwestern
Journal of Anthropology (Journal of Anthropological Research) 28 (4):299-332.

Kuhn, Thomas
The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, Second edition. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.

Kuznar, Lawrence A.
1997 Reclaiming a Scientific Anthropology. Walnut Creek, Ca: AltaMira.
Lave, Jean
1988 Cognition in Practice. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Layton, Robert
An Introduction to Theory in Anthropology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Newman, Katherine S.
Falling from Grace: The Experience of Downward Mobility in the American
Middle Class. New York: Free Press.
Declining Fortunes: The Withering of the American Dream. New York: Basic
Books.
Deindustrialization, Poverty, and Downward Mobility: Toward an Anthropology
of Economic Disorder. IN Diagnosing America: Anthropology and Public Engagement. Edited by Shepard Forman. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press. Pages 121-148.

Pálsson, Gísli
Enskilment at Sea. Man 29 (4):901-927).
The Textual Life of Savants: Ethnography, Iceland, and the Linguistic Turn. Chur, Switzerland: Harwood Academic Publishers.

Romney, A. Kimball, Susan C. Weller, and William H. Batchelder
Culture as Consensus: A Theory of Culture and Informant Accuracy. American Anthropologist 88(2):313-338.

Romney, A. Kimball, Carmella Morre, and Craig D. Rush
Cultural Universals: Measuring the Semantic Structure of Emotion Terms
English and Japanese. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 94:5489-
5494.

Romney, A. Kimball and Carmella Moore
1998 Toward a Theory of Culture as Shared Cognitive Structures. Ethos 26(3):314-337.

Rubin, Lillian B.
Worlds of Pain: Life in the Working-Class Family. New York: Basic Books.
Van Esterik, Penny
1978 Comment on Brown et al. American Ethnologist 5(2):404-405.

Wallace, Anthony F.C.
Culture and Personality. New York: Random House.

Weller, Susan C. and A. Kimball Romney
1988 Systematic Data Collection. Newbury Park: Sage.

SUPERVISOR MANAGER STEWARD UNION REP DIF. LINE
MANAGER 1.0
STEWARD 0.0 0.0
UNION REP 0.0 0.0 1.0
DIF. LINE 0.0 0.0 0.5 0.5
SAME LINE 0.0 0.0 0.5 0.5 1.0TABLE 1
PERFECT UNION MODEL

SCH96 STAFF INDUST PUBLIC STAFF2 PURE
SCH98 -.41 -.47 -.18 -.32 -.39 -.39
SCH96 .64 -.16 .68 .64 .67
STAFF .29 .89 .98 .96
INDUST .14 .17 .21
PUBLIC .88 .88
STAFF2 .96
TABLE 2
CORRELATIONS AMONG PROXIMITY MATRICES

SUPERVISOR MANAGER STEWARD UNION REP DIF. LINE
MANAGER .73
STEWARD .23 .18
UNION REP .09 .25 .89
DIF. LINE .16 .25 .18 .27
SAME LINE .25 .20 .27 .18 .82TABLE 3
INDUSTRIAL MEMBERS
SUPERVISOR MANAGER STEWARD UNION REP DIF. LINE
MANAGER .93
STEWARD .10 .08
UNION REP .18 .25 .88
DIF. LINE .10 .08 .35 .28
SAME LINE .05 .08 .48 .30 .90TABLE 4
PUBLIC HOSPITAL MEMBERS

SUPERVISOR MANAGER STEWARD UNION REP DIF. LINE
MANAGER .83
STEWARD .04 .06
UNION REP .00 .07 .68
DIF. LINE .04 .06 .64 .49
SAME LINE .08 .03 .73 .44 .94TABLE 5
STAFF
SUPERVISOR MANAGER STEWARD UNION REP DIF. LINE
MANAGER .81
STEWARD .13 .06
UNION REP .10 .07 .60
DIF. LINE .28 .22 .31 .17
SAME LINE .31 .26 .26 .26 .88

TABLE 6
SCHWAB 1996

SUPERVISOR MANAGER STEWARD UNION REP DIF. LINE
MANAGER .86
STEWARD .32 .22
UNION REP .28 .23 .70
DIF. LINE .17 .19 .19 .22
SAME LINE .21 .28 .18 .22 .84TABLE 7
SCHWAB 1998
SITE N RATIO
SCHWAB96 6 4.5
SCHWAB98 25 4.0
STAFF 18 5.8
STAFF2 25 5.2
PUBLIC 9 10.6
IUDUSTRIAL 11 11.2
TABLE 8
SITES, NUMBERS OF RESPONDANTS, AND RATIO OF FIRST AND SECOND FACTORS IN CONSENSUS ANALYSIS
WORKER

STEWARD SUPERVISOR
MANAGER
REP HUMAN RELATIONS DIRECTOR

VP FOR HUMAN RELATIONS

FIGURE 1
ETIC GRID

DLWORKER
SLWORKER
SUPERVISOR
MANAGER REP

STEWARD
FIGURE 2
SCHWAB 1996

DL WORKERSUPERVISOR

MANAGER
SL WORK

STEWARD

UNION REP
FIGURE 3
PUBLIC HOSPITAL

DLWORKER
SLWORKER

STEWARD

REP MANAGER
SUPERVISOR

FIGURE 4
SCHWAB 1998
REP/STEWARDS MAN/SUPER

WORKERS

FIGURE 5
INDUSTRIAL MEMBERS
WORKERS
SUPERVISOR STEWARD

MANAGER REP
FIGURE 6
STAFF